The motor-semantics coupling effect in second language writing
BAI Yating1,2, QIN Nan3, HE Wenguang1
1 School of Psychology, Qufu Normal University, Qufu 273165; 2 Department of Psychology, Renmin University, Beijing 100872; 3 Mental Health Education Center, Shandong Agricultural University Taian 271018
白亚停, 秦楠, 何文广. 二语书写中的运动——语义耦合效应[J]. 心理研究, 2025, 18(3): 213-220.
BAI Yating, QIN Nan, HE Wenguang. The motor-semantics coupling effect in second language writing. Psychological Research, 2025, 18(3): 213-220.
[1] 王成, 尤文平, 张清芳.(2012).书写产生过程的认知机制.心理科学进展, 20(10), 1560-1572. [2] Afonso O., Suárez‐Coalla P., Cuetos F., Ibáñez A., Sedeño L., & García A. M. (2019). The embodied penman: Effector‐specific motor-language integration during handwriting.Cognitive Science, 43(7), e12767. [3] Bergen B., Lau T. T., Narayan S., Stojanovic D., & Wheeler K. (2012). Body part representations in verbal semantics.Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 969-981. [4] Dalla Volta R., Fabbri-Destro M., Gentilucci M., & Avanzini P. (2014). Spatiotemporal dynamics during processing of abstract and concrete verbs: An ERP study.Neuropsychologia, 61, 163-174. [5] Diefenbach C., Rieger M., Massen C., & Prinz W. (2013). Action-sentence compatibility: The role of action effects and timing.Frontiers in Psychology, 4(4), 272. [6] Fargier R., Menoret M., Boulenger V., Nazir T. A., & Paulignan Y. (2012). Grasp it loudly! Supporting actions with semantically congruent spoken action words.PLoS One, 7, e30663. [7] Galen, G. V. (1991). Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory.Human Movement Science, 10(2-3), 165-191. [8] García, A. M., & Ibáñez, A. (2016a). A touch with words: Dynamic synergies between manual actions and language. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 59-95. [9] García, A. M., & Ibánez, A. (2016b). Hands typing what hands do: Action-semantic integration dynamics throughout written verb production. Cognition, 149, 56-66. [10] García-Marco, Morera Y., D Beltrán, Vega, M. D., Herrera E., & Sedeño,et al. (2019). Negation markers inhibit motor routines during typing of manual action verbs.Cognition, 182, 286-293. [11] Kaschak, M. P., & Borreggine, K. L. (2008). Temporal dynamics of the action-sentence compatibility effect.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,(Hove) 61, 883-895. [12] Kogan B., Garciá-Marco E., Birba A., Cortes C., Melloni M., Ibáñez A., & Garciá A. M. (2020). How words ripple through bilingual hands: Motor-language coupling during L1 and L2 writing.Neuropsychologia, 146, 107563. [13] Pacini, A. M., & Barnard, P. J. (2021). Exocentric coding of the mapping between valence and regions of space: Implications for embodied cognition.Acta Psychologica, 214(4), 103264. [14] Spadacenta S., Gallese V., Fragola M., & Mirabella G. (2014). Modulation of arm reaching movements during processing of arm/hand-related action verbs with and without emotional connotation.PLoS One, 9, e104349. [15] Tomasino B., Werner C. J., Weiss P. H., & Fink G. R. (2007). Stimulus properties matter more than perspective: An fMRI study of mental imagery and silent reading of action phrases.Neuroimage, 36, 128-141. [16] Vukovic, N. (2013). When words get physical: Evidence for proficiency-modulated somatotopic motor interference during second language comprehension.Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 35(35).