The influence of promise characteristics on primary school children’s development of promise fulfilling: The effect of benefit loss and inhibitory control
DING Fang1, ZHANG Xinyuan1, LIU Jiao1,2
1 Department of Psychology, School of Education, Soochow University, Suzhou 215123; 2 Hangzhou Shangsi Middle School of Zhejiang Province, Hangzhou 310008
丁芳, 张欣媛, 刘娇. 承诺特征对小学儿童承诺履行发展的影响:利益损失和抑制控制的作用[J]. 心理研究, 2023, 16(6): 502-511.
DING Fang, ZHANG Xinyuan, LIU Jiao. The influence of promise characteristics on primary school children’s development of promise fulfilling: The effect of benefit loss and inhibitory control. Psychological Research, 2023, 16(6): 502-511.
[1] 冯彪, 李宗龙, 王凯欣, 崔洪波. (2022). 大学生生活目标与心理韧性的关系: 自我控制和一般自我效能感的多重中介作用.心理研究, 15(1), 78-85. [2] 耿希峰, 马丽枝, 隋光远. (2009). 7~11岁儿童分享行为动机分析.心理科学, 32(6), 1355-1358. [3] 刘娟, 李红. (2010). 5~8岁儿童二级信念-愿望推理能力的发展.心理发展与教育, 26(3), 233-238. [4] 李贺, 莫雷, 罗秋铃, 莫然, 俞梦霞, 黎沛昕, 衷禾. (2014). 签名对个体诚实度和道德感的影响.心理学报, 46(9), 1347-1354. [5] 李明苹, 卢家楣. (2022). 累觉不爱? 基于自我损耗降低未来乐观期望的研究证据.心理研究, 15(2), 126-131. [6] 林佳燕, 傅根跃. (2010). 承诺对学前儿童违规及相关行为的影响.心理科学, 33(5), 1118-1121. [7] 马钰, 肖晨洁, 车敬上, 王海侠, 李爱梅. (2020). 自我损耗降低决策理性: 心理机制与边界条件.心理科学进展, 28(11), 1911-1925. [8] 聂衍刚, 利振华, 窦凯, 毛兰平. (2018). 累了会说谎? 自我损耗削弱诚信行为及其缓解机制.心理与行为研究, 16(4), 525-533. [9] 皮亚杰, J. (1980). 儿童的语言与思维. (傅统先译). 北京: 文化教育出版社. [10] 皮亚杰, J. (1984). 儿童的道德判断. (傅统先, 陆有铨译). 济南: 山东教育出版社. [11] 任旭东. (1995, 8月). 论承诺.政法论坛, (4), 37-41. [12] 王彤文. (2016, 5月). 在利益损失情况下小学生诚信承诺与不诚信行为的实验研究.课程教育研究, (15), 210-212. [13] Baddeley A., Logie R., Bressi S., Sala S. D., & Spinnler H. (1986). Dementia and working memory.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(4), 603-618. [14] Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497-529. [15] Belot M., Bhaskar V., & van de Ven, J. (2010). Promises and cooperation: Evidence from a TV game show.Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 73(3), 396-405. [16] Bensefia A., Paquet T., & Heutte L. (2005). Handwritten document analysis for automatic writer recognition.Electronic Letters on Computer Vision and Image Analysis, 5(2), 72-86. [17] Bernicot, J., & Laval, V. (1996). Promises in French children: Comprehension and metapragmatic knowledge.Journal of Pragmatics, 25(1), 101-122. [18] Bussey, K. (2010). The role of promises for children’s trustworthiness and honesty. In K. J. Rotenberg (Ed.), Interpersonal trust during childhood and adolescence (pp. 155-176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [19] Carlsson F., Kataria M., Krupnick A., Lampi E., Lofgren A., Qin P., & Sterner T. (2013). The truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth — A multiple country test of an oath script.Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 89, 105-121. [20] Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership.Econometrica, 74(6), 1579-1601. [21] Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2010). Bare promises: An experiment.Economics Letters, 107(2), 281-283. [22] Chen, Y., & Zhang, Y. (2021). Do elicited promises affect people’s trust? Observations in the trust game experiment.Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 93(3), 101726. [23] Chin, J. -C., & Lin, M.-H. (2018). Children’s understanding of conditional promise contract violations.Infant and Child Development, 27(3), e2082. [24] Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task.Cognition, 31(3), 187-276. [25] Ding X. P., Omrin D. S., Evans A. D., Fu G., Chen G., & Lee K. (2014). Elementary school children’s cheating behavior and its cognitive correlates.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 85-95. [26] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [27] Friese M., Engeler M., & Florack A. (2015). Self-perceived successful weight regulators are less affected by self-regulatory depletion in the domain of eating behavior.Eating Behaviors, 16, 5-8. [28] Frith, C. D., & Singer, T. (2008). The role of social cognition in decision making.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 3875-3886. [29] Fu G., Heyman G. D., Qian M., Guo T., & Lee K. (2015). Young children with a positive reputation to maintain are less likely to cheat.Developmental Science, 19(2), 275-283. [30] Harris, P. L., & Núñez, M. (1996). Understanding of permission rules by preschool children.Child Development, 67(4), 1572-1591. [31] Heyman G. D., Fu G., Lin J., Qian M. K., & Lee K. (2015). Eliciting promises from children reduces cheating.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 242-248. [32] Jacquemet N., Joule R. V., Luchini S., & Shogren J. F. (2013). Preference elicitation under oath.Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 65(1), 110-132. [33] Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk.Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. [34] Kalish, C. W., & Cornelius, R. (2007). What is to be done? Children’s ascriptions of conventional obligations.Child Development, 78(3), 859-878. [35] Kam M., Gummadidala K., Fielding G., & Conn R. (2001). Signature authentication by forensic document examiners.Journal of Forensic Sciences, 46(4), 884-888. [36] Kanngiesser P., Köymen B., & Tomasello M. (2017). Young children mostly keep, and expect others to keep, their promises.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 140-158. [37] Kettle, K. L., & Häubl, G. (2011). The signature effect: Signing influences consumption-related behavior by priming self-identity.Journal of Consumer Research, 38(3), 474-489. [38] Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row. [39] Lee, J. A., & Murnighan, J. K. (2001). The empathy-prospect model and the choice to help.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31(4), 816-839. [40] Luking K. R., Pagliaccio D., Luby J. L., & Barch D. M. (2016). Do losses loom larger for children than adults?Emotion, 16(3), 338-348. [41] Mant, C. M., & Perner, J. (1988). The child’s understanding of commitment.Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 343-351. [42] Nilsen, E. S., & Valcke, A. (2018). Children’s sharing with collaborators versus competitors: The impact of theory of mind and executive functioning.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 58, 38-48. [43] Pliszka S. R., Borcherding S. H., Spratley K., Leon S., & Irick S. (1997). Measuring inhibitory control in children.Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 18(4), 254-259. [44] Rosenhan, D. L. (1972). Learning theory and prosocial behavior.Journal of Social Issues, 28(3), 151-163. [45] Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1985). Temperament and the development of self-regulation. New York: Springer. [46] Shu L. L., Mazar N., Gino F., Ariely D., & Bazerman M. H. (2012). Signing at the beginning makes ethics salient and decreases dishonest self-reports in comparison to signing at the end.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(38), 15197-15200. [47] Steinbeis N., Bernhardt B. C., & Singer T. (2012). Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior.Neuron, 73(5), 1040-1051. [48] Talwar V., Lee K., Bala N., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (2004). Children’s lie-telling to conceal a parent’s transgression: Legal implications.Law and Human Behavior, 28(4), 411-435. [49] van den Bos W., Cohen M. X., Kahnt T., & Crone E. A. (2012). Striatum-medial prefrontal cortex connectivity predicts developmental changes in reinforcement learning.Cerebral Cortex, 22(6), 1247-1255. [50] Vogel E. K., McCollough A. W., & Machizawa M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory.Nature, 438(7067), 500-503. [51] Zhao L., Heyman G. D., Chen L., & Lee K. (2017). Telling young children they have a reputation for being smart promotes cheating.Developmental Science, 21(3), e12585.