[1] 吴彦文, 游旭群, 李海霞. (2014). 注意力资源限制与双任务的相互干扰机制.心理学报, 46(2), 174-184. [2] 吴彦文, 游旭群. (2007). 双任务情境下心理旋转的并行加工机制.心理学报, 39(5), 785-794. [3] 张学民. (2007). 实验心理学. 北京: 北京师范大学出版社,271-272. [4] 章鹏, 张琪涵, 彭国慧, 宋星, 白学军. (2016). 汉语双字词在心理词典中的表征方式: 来自fNIRS的证据.心理科学, 39(4), 849-855. [5] Gu J., Li X., & Liversedge S. P. (2015). Character order processing in Chinese reading.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 127-137. [6] Hoffmann M. A., Pieczykolan A., Koch I., & Huestegge L. (2020). Two sources of task prioritization: The interplay of effector-based and task order-based capacity allocation in the PRP paradigm.Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 82(7), 3402-3414. [7] Katus, T., & Eimer, M. (2019). The sources of dual-task costs in multisensory working memory tasks.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(2), 175-185. [8] Klapp S. T., Maslovat D., & Jagacinski R. J. (2019). The bottleneck of the psychological refractory period effect involves timing of response initiation rather than response selection.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(1), 29-47. [9] Leonhard T., Fernández S. R., Ulrich R., & Miller J. (2011). Dual-task processing when task 1 is hard and task 2 is easy: Reversed central processing order? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(1), 115-136. [10] Levy J., Pashler H., & Boer E. (2006). Central interference in driving: Is there any stopping the psychological refractory period? Psychological Science, 17(3), 228-235. [11] Miller, J., & Durst, M. (2015). A comparison of the psychological refractory period and prioritized processing paradigms: Can the response-selection bottleneck model explain them both? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(5), 1420-1441. [12] Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory.Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220-244. [13] Pashler H., Harris C. R., & Nuechterlein K. H. (2008). Does the central bottleneck encompass voluntary selection of hedonically-based choices? Experimental Psychology, 55(5), 313-321. [14] Schubert, T., & Strobach, T. (2018). Practice-related optimization of dual-task performance: Efficient task instantiation during overlapping task processing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(12), 1884-1904. [15] Strobach T., Hendrich E., Kübler S., Müller H., & Schubert T. (2018). Processing order in dual-task situations: The “first-come, first-served” principle and the impact of task order instructions.Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(7), 1785-1803. [16] Strobach T., Kübler S., & Schubert T. (2019). Endogenous control of task-order preparation in variable dual tasks.Psychological Research, 85(1), 345-363. [17] Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14(1), 1-36. [18] Töllner T., Strobach T., Torsten S. T., & Müller H. J. (2012). The effect of task order predictability in audio-visual dual task performance: Just a central capacity limitation? Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 75-87. [19] Tombu, M., & Jolic? ur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 3-18. [20] Tombu, M., & Jolicœur, P. (2005). Testing the predictions of the central capacity sharing model.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(4), 790-802. [21] White, D., & Besner, D. (2018). Attentional constraints on semantic activation: Evidence from Stroop’s paradigm.Acta Psychologica, 189, 4-11. [22] Wu, C., & Liu, Y. (2008). Queuing network modeling of the psychological refractory period (PRP).Psychological Review, 115(4), 913-954.