王丽君, 李永鑫, 赵国祥. 错误后调整效应中的争议:功能适应性和任务特异性[J]. 心理研究, 2022, 15(5): 403-410.
WANG Lijun, LI Yongxin, ZHAO Guoxiang. The controversies in the post-error adjustment effect:Functional adaptability and task specificity. Psychological Research, 2022, 15(5): 403-410.
[1] 王丽君, 胡学平, 索涛, 赵国祥, 陈安涛. (2019). 脑岛自发神经活动强度可预测个体错误后反应调整速度. 科学通报, 64(21), 2207-2215. [2] 王丽君, 刘长平, 胡学平, 陈安涛. (2016). 警觉水平影响错误后行为适应.科学通报, (34), 3708-3717. [3] 王丽君, 索涛, 赵国祥. (2020). 未意识到错误影响错误后调整的电生理证据.心理学报, 52(10), 1189-1198. [4] 王丽君, 徐雷, 伍姗姗, 谭金凤, 陈安涛. (2013). 错误后减慢理论模型述评.心理科学进展, 21(3), 418-428. Aron, A. R., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S., Frank, M. J., & Poldrack, R. A.(14), 3743-3752. [5] Botvinick M. M., Braver T. S., Barch D. M., Carter C. S., & Cohen J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control.Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652. [6] Buzzell G. A., Beatty P. J., Paquette N. A., Roberts D. M., & McDonald C. G.(2017). Error-induced blindness: Error detection leads to impaired sensory processing and lower accuracy at short response-stimulus intervals.The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(11), 2895-2903. [7] Cho R. Y., Orr J. M., Cohen J. D., & Carter C. S. (2009). Generalized signaling for control: Evidence from postconflict and posterror performance adjustments.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1161-1177. [8] Coleman J. R., Watson J. M., & Strayer D. L. (2018). Working memory capacity and task goals modulate error‐related ERPs.Psychophysiology, 55(3), e12805. [9] Collins, A. G. E., & Frank, M. J. (2018). Within- and across-trial dynamics of human EEG reveal cooperative interplay between reinforcement learning and working memory.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(10), 2502-2507. [10] Crowley M. J., Wu J., Bailey C. A., & Mayes L. C. (2009). Bringing in the negative reinforcements: The avoidance feedback-related negativity.Neuroreport, 20(17), 1513-1517. [11] Damaso K., Williams P., & Heathcote A. (2020). Evidence for different types of errors being associated with different types of post-error changes.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(3), 435-440. [12] Danielmeier C., Eichele T., Forstmann B. U., Tittgemeyer M., & Ullsperger M. (2011). Posterior medial frontal cortex activity predicts post-error adaptations in task-related visual and motor areas.Journal of Neuroscience, 31(5), 1780-1789. [13] Dutilh G., Vandekerckhove J., Forstmann B. U., Keuleers E., Brysbaert M., & Wagenmakers E. J. (2012). Testing theories of post-error slowing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 1-12. [14] Fischer A. G., Nigbur R., Klein T. A., Danielmeier C., & Ullsperger M. (2018). Cortical beta power reflects decision dynamics and uncovers multiple facets of post-error adaptation.Nature Communications, 9(1), 5038. [15] Forster, S. E., & Cho, R. Y. (2014). Context specificity of post-error and post-conflict cognitive control adjustments.PLOS ONE, 9(3), e90281. [16] Guan, Y., & Wessel, J. R. (2021). Timing‐dependent differential effects of unexpected events on error processing reveal the interactive dynamics of surprise and error processing.Psychophysiology, 58(12), e13922. [17] Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity.Psychological Review, 109(4), 679-709. [18] Houtman, F., & Notebaert, W. (2013). Blinded by error.Cognition, 128(2), 228-236. [19] Jentzsch, I., & Dudschig, C. (2009). Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(2), 209-218. [20] Laming D. R.J. (1968). Information theory of choice-reaction times. London, UK: Academic Press. [21] Lavro D, Ben Shachar M S., Saville C W N, Klein C., & Berger A. (2018). Testing the bottleneck account for post-error slowing beyond the post-error response.Biological Psychology, 138, 81-90. [22] Li Q., Hu N., Li Y., Long Q., Gu Y., Tang Y., & Chen A. (2020). Error-induced adaptability: Behavioral and neural dynamics of response-stimulus interval modulations on posterror slowing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(5), 851-863. Li, Q., Long, Q., Hu, N., Tang, Y., & Chen, A., 370. [23] Liu, C., & Huo, Z. (2020). A tradeoff relationship between internal monitoring and external feedback during the dynamic process of reinforcement learning - ScienceDirect.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 150, 11-19. [24] Maier M. E., Ernst B., & Steinhauser M. (2019). Error-related pupil dilation is sensitive to the evaluation of different error types.Biological Psychology, 141, 25-34. [25] Maier M. E., Yeung N., & Steinhauser M. (2011). Error-related brain activity and adjustments of selective attention following errors.NeuroImage, 56(4), 2339-2347. [26] Notebaert W., Houtman F., Van Opstal F., Gevers W., Fias W., & Verguts T. (2009). Post-error slowing: An orienting account.Cognition, 111(2), 275-279. [27] Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2011). Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task.Acta Psychol (Amst), 136(2), 212-216. [28] Purcell, B. A., & Kiani, R. (2016). Neural mechanisms of post-error adjustments of decision policy in parietal cortex.Neuron, 89(3), 658-671. [29] Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(2), 264-272. [30] Steinhauser M., Ernst B., & Ibald K. W. (2017). Isolating component processes of posterror slowing with the psychological refractory period Paradigm.Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(4), 653-659. [31] Steinhauser R., Maier M. E., & Steinhauser M. (2017). Neural signatures of adaptive post-error adjustments in visual search.NeuroImage, 150, 270-278. [32] Tan Y., Vandeput J., Qiu J., Bergh O. V. d., & Leupoldt A. v. (2019). The error-related negativity for error processing in interoception.NeuroImage, 184, 386-395. [33] Ullsperger, M., & Danielmeier, C. (2016). Reducing speed and sight: How adaptive is post-error slowing?Neuron, 89(3), 430-432. [34] Ullsperger M., Danielmeier C., & Jocham G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior.Physiological Reviews, 94(1), 35-79. [35] Van der Borght L., Braem S., Stevens M., & Notebaert W. (2016). Keep calm and be patient: The influence of anxiety and time on post-error adaptations.Acta Psychologica, 164, 34-38. [36] Wang L., Pan W., Tan J., Liu C., & Chen A. (2016). Slowing after observed error transfers across tasks.PLOS ONE, 11(3), e0149836. [37] Wessel, J. R. (2017). An adaptive orienting theory of error processing. Psychophysiology, e13041. [38] Wessel, J. R., & Aron, A. R. (2017). On the globality of motor suppression: Unexpected events and their influence on behavior and cognition.Neuron, 93(2), 259-280. [39] Wessel J. R., Danielmeier C., & Ullsperger M. (2011). Error awareness revisited: Accumulation of multimodal evidence from central and autonomic nervous systems.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 3021-3036. [40] Wessel J. R., Jenkinson N., Brittain J. S., Voets S. H., Aziz T. Z., & & Aron, A. R. (2016). Surprise disrupts cognition via a frontobasal ganglia suppressive mechanism.Nature Communications, 7, 11195. [41] Wessel J. R., Jiang J., & Stolley J. J. (2022). Action errors impair active working memory maintenance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. [42] Zhuang Q., Zhu S., Yang X., Zhou X., Xu X., Chen Z.,.. Kendrick K. M. (2020). Oxytocin-induced facilitation of learning in a probabilistic task is associated with reduced feedback-and error-related negativity potentials. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 1-10.