|
|
The influence of promise characteristics on primary school children’s development of promise fulfilling: The effect of benefit loss and inhibitory control |
DING Fang1, ZHANG Xinyuan1, LIU Jiao1,2 |
1 Department of Psychology, School of Education, Soochow University, Suzhou 215123; 2 Hangzhou Shangsi Middle School of Zhejiang Province, Hangzhou 310008 |
|
|
Abstract The aim of this research was to explore the development and influencing factors of primary school children’s promise fulfilling. In experiment 1, by adopting the dictator game paradigm, a total of 128 primary school children aged 7, 9 and 11 years old were recruited to investigate the influence of promise methods on primary school children’s promise fulfilling under the condition of benefit loss. In experiment 2, by adopting the stop-signal task and dictator game paradigm, a total of 144 primary school children aged 7, 9 and 11 years old were recruited to investigate the influence of promise voluntary on primary school children’s promise fulfilling under the high/low inhibitory control levels. The results were as follows: (1) 9 years old was the turning age when primary school children began to fulfill promises; (2) Primary school children were easier to fulfill promises in the situations with than without benefit loss. Written promise was better than oral promise and no promise in promoting the primary school children’s promise fulfilling. The influence of promise methods on primary school children’s promise fulfilling was different in two benefit loss situations. Specifically, primary school children who adopted oral promise and no promise were more likely to fulfill promises in the benefit loss situation, whereas the difference for primary school children’s promise fulfilling with written promise was not significant in the situations with or without benefit loss; (3) Primary school children with high inhibitory control level were more inclined to fulfill promises. Primary school children who adopted voluntary promise were more willing to fulfill promises. In conclusion, the research indicates that there is a turning age in the development of primary school children’s promise fulfilling and promise fulfilling is affected by promise characteristics, benefit loss, and inhibitory control.
|
|
|
|
|
[1] 冯彪, 李宗龙, 王凯欣, 崔洪波. (2022). 大学生生活目标与心理韧性的关系: 自我控制和一般自我效能感的多重中介作用.心理研究, 15(1), 78-85. [2] 耿希峰, 马丽枝, 隋光远. (2009). 7~11岁儿童分享行为动机分析.心理科学, 32(6), 1355-1358. [3] 刘娟, 李红. (2010). 5~8岁儿童二级信念-愿望推理能力的发展.心理发展与教育, 26(3), 233-238. [4] 李贺, 莫雷, 罗秋铃, 莫然, 俞梦霞, 黎沛昕, 衷禾. (2014). 签名对个体诚实度和道德感的影响.心理学报, 46(9), 1347-1354. [5] 李明苹, 卢家楣. (2022). 累觉不爱? 基于自我损耗降低未来乐观期望的研究证据.心理研究, 15(2), 126-131. [6] 林佳燕, 傅根跃. (2010). 承诺对学前儿童违规及相关行为的影响.心理科学, 33(5), 1118-1121. [7] 马钰, 肖晨洁, 车敬上, 王海侠, 李爱梅. (2020). 自我损耗降低决策理性: 心理机制与边界条件.心理科学进展, 28(11), 1911-1925. [8] 聂衍刚, 利振华, 窦凯, 毛兰平. (2018). 累了会说谎? 自我损耗削弱诚信行为及其缓解机制.心理与行为研究, 16(4), 525-533. [9] 皮亚杰, J. (1980). 儿童的语言与思维. (傅统先译). 北京: 文化教育出版社. [10] 皮亚杰, J. (1984). 儿童的道德判断. (傅统先, 陆有铨译). 济南: 山东教育出版社. [11] 任旭东. (1995, 8月). 论承诺.政法论坛, (4), 37-41. [12] 王彤文. (2016, 5月). 在利益损失情况下小学生诚信承诺与不诚信行为的实验研究.课程教育研究, (15), 210-212. [13] Baddeley A., Logie R., Bressi S., Sala S. D., & Spinnler H. (1986). Dementia and working memory.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(4), 603-618. [14] Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497-529. [15] Belot M., Bhaskar V., & van de Ven, J. (2010). Promises and cooperation: Evidence from a TV game show.Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 73(3), 396-405. [16] Bensefia A., Paquet T., & Heutte L. (2005). Handwritten document analysis for automatic writer recognition.Electronic Letters on Computer Vision and Image Analysis, 5(2), 72-86. [17] Bernicot, J., & Laval, V. (1996). Promises in French children: Comprehension and metapragmatic knowledge.Journal of Pragmatics, 25(1), 101-122. [18] Bussey, K. (2010). The role of promises for children’s trustworthiness and honesty. In K. J. Rotenberg (Ed.), Interpersonal trust during childhood and adolescence (pp. 155-176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [19] Carlsson F., Kataria M., Krupnick A., Lampi E., Lofgren A., Qin P., & Sterner T. (2013). The truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth — A multiple country test of an oath script.Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 89, 105-121. [20] Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership.Econometrica, 74(6), 1579-1601. [21] Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2010). Bare promises: An experiment.Economics Letters, 107(2), 281-283. [22] Chen, Y., & Zhang, Y. (2021). Do elicited promises affect people’s trust? Observations in the trust game experiment.Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 93(3), 101726. [23] Chin, J. -C., & Lin, M.-H. (2018). Children’s understanding of conditional promise contract violations.Infant and Child Development, 27(3), e2082. [24] Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task.Cognition, 31(3), 187-276. [25] Ding X. P., Omrin D. S., Evans A. D., Fu G., Chen G., & Lee K. (2014). Elementary school children’s cheating behavior and its cognitive correlates.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 85-95. [26] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [27] Friese M., Engeler M., & Florack A. (2015). Self-perceived successful weight regulators are less affected by self-regulatory depletion in the domain of eating behavior.Eating Behaviors, 16, 5-8. [28] Frith, C. D., & Singer, T. (2008). The role of social cognition in decision making.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 3875-3886. [29] Fu G., Heyman G. D., Qian M., Guo T., & Lee K. (2015). Young children with a positive reputation to maintain are less likely to cheat.Developmental Science, 19(2), 275-283. [30] Harris, P. L., & Núñez, M. (1996). Understanding of permission rules by preschool children.Child Development, 67(4), 1572-1591. [31] Heyman G. D., Fu G., Lin J., Qian M. K., & Lee K. (2015). Eliciting promises from children reduces cheating.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 242-248. [32] Jacquemet N., Joule R. V., Luchini S., & Shogren J. F. (2013). Preference elicitation under oath.Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 65(1), 110-132. [33] Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk.Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. [34] Kalish, C. W., & Cornelius, R. (2007). What is to be done? Children’s ascriptions of conventional obligations.Child Development, 78(3), 859-878. [35] Kam M., Gummadidala K., Fielding G., & Conn R. (2001). Signature authentication by forensic document examiners.Journal of Forensic Sciences, 46(4), 884-888. [36] Kanngiesser P., Köymen B., & Tomasello M. (2017). Young children mostly keep, and expect others to keep, their promises.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 140-158. [37] Kettle, K. L., & Häubl, G. (2011). The signature effect: Signing influences consumption-related behavior by priming self-identity.Journal of Consumer Research, 38(3), 474-489. [38] Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row. [39] Lee, J. A., & Murnighan, J. K. (2001). The empathy-prospect model and the choice to help.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31(4), 816-839. [40] Luking K. R., Pagliaccio D., Luby J. L., & Barch D. M. (2016). Do losses loom larger for children than adults?Emotion, 16(3), 338-348. [41] Mant, C. M., & Perner, J. (1988). The child’s understanding of commitment.Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 343-351. [42] Nilsen, E. S., & Valcke, A. (2018). Children’s sharing with collaborators versus competitors: The impact of theory of mind and executive functioning.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 58, 38-48. [43] Pliszka S. R., Borcherding S. H., Spratley K., Leon S., & Irick S. (1997). Measuring inhibitory control in children.Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 18(4), 254-259. [44] Rosenhan, D. L. (1972). Learning theory and prosocial behavior.Journal of Social Issues, 28(3), 151-163. [45] Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1985). Temperament and the development of self-regulation. New York: Springer. [46] Shu L. L., Mazar N., Gino F., Ariely D., & Bazerman M. H. (2012). Signing at the beginning makes ethics salient and decreases dishonest self-reports in comparison to signing at the end.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(38), 15197-15200. [47] Steinbeis N., Bernhardt B. C., & Singer T. (2012). Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior.Neuron, 73(5), 1040-1051. [48] Talwar V., Lee K., Bala N., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (2004). Children’s lie-telling to conceal a parent’s transgression: Legal implications.Law and Human Behavior, 28(4), 411-435. [49] van den Bos W., Cohen M. X., Kahnt T., & Crone E. A. (2012). Striatum-medial prefrontal cortex connectivity predicts developmental changes in reinforcement learning.Cerebral Cortex, 22(6), 1247-1255. [50] Vogel E. K., McCollough A. W., & Machizawa M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory.Nature, 438(7067), 500-503. [51] Zhao L., Heyman G. D., Chen L., & Lee K. (2017). Telling young children they have a reputation for being smart promotes cheating.Developmental Science, 21(3), e12585. |
|
|
|