The influence of emotions and global-local processing on creative thinking
ZHANG Fengjuan1,2, HONG Lingzhi3, ZHANG Zhonglu4
1 Department of Psychology, College of Education, Guangxi Normal University, Guilin 541004; 2 Guangxi Colleges and Universities Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Applied Psychology, Guilin 541004; 3 School of Education Science, Guangxi Minzu University, Nanning 530006; 4 School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou 510006
[1] 杜秀敏. (2012). 汉字远距离联想问题解决的特点研究. 中国科学院研究生院博士学位论文. [2] 胡卫平, 王博韬, 段海军, 程丽芳, 周寰, 李晶晶. (2015). 情绪影响创造性认知过程的神经机制.心理科学进展, 23(11), 1869-1878. [3] 靳霄, 邓光辉, 经旻, 林国志. (2009). 视频材料诱发情绪的效果评价.心理学探新, 29(6), 83-87. [4] 卢家楣, 刘伟, 贺雯, 卢盛华. (2002). 情绪状态对学生创造性的影响.心理学报, 34(4), 381-386. [5] 沈汪兵, 刘昌, 施春华, 袁媛. (2015). 创造性思维的性别差异.心理科学进展, 23(8), 1380-1389. [6] 石婷婷. (2017). 愤怒情绪影响创造性认知过程的fNIRS研究. 陕西师范大学硕士学位论文. [7] Ashby, F. G., & Isen, A. M. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition.Psychological Review, 106(3), 529-550. [8] Biss, R. K., & Hasher, L. (2011). Delighted and distracted: Positive affect increases priming for irrelevant information.Emotion, 11(6), 1474-1478. [9] Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2009). How the object of affect guides its impact.Emotion Review, 1(1), 39-54. [10] Damian, R. I., & Robins, R. W. (2012). The link between dispositional pride and creative thinking depends on current mood.Journal of Research in Personality, 46(6), 765-769. [11] De Dreu C. K., Baas M., & Nijstad B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model.Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 94(5), 739-756. [12] Eastwood J. D., Frischen A., Fenske M. J., & Smilek D. (2012). The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention.Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482-495. [13] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., &?Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [14] Fernández-Abascal, E. G., & Díaz, M. D. M. (2013). Affective induction and creative thinking.Creativity Research Journal, 25(2), 213-221. [15] Förster, J., & Dannenberg, L. (2010). GLOMOsys: A systems account of global versus local processing.Psychological Inquiry, 21(3), 175-197. [16] Förster J., Liberman N., & Kuschel S. (2008). The effect of global versus local processing styles on assimilation versus contrast in social judgment.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 579-599. [17] Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions.American Psychologist, 56(3), 218. [18] Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires.Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313-332. [19] Gasper, K., & Clore, G. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information.Psychological Science, 13(1), 34-40. [20] Gross, J, J., Levenson, & W, R. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films.Cognition & Emotion, 9(1), 87-108. [21] Guilford. (1967). The nature of human intelLigence. McGrawHill. [22] Hao, N. (2010). The effects of domain knowledge and instructional manipulation on creative idea generation.The Journal of Creative Behavior, 44(4), 237-257. [23] Hao N., Lu K., & Cheng R. (2021). The effect of anger on malevolent creativity and strategies for its emotion regulation.Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(8), 847-860. [24] Huntsinger, J. R., & Ray, C. (2016). A flexible influence of affective feelings on creative and analytic performance.Emotion, 16(6), 826-837. [25] Huntsinger J. R., Clore G. L., & Bar-Anan Y. (2010). Mood and global-local focus: Priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing.Emotion, 10(5), 722-726. [26] Huntsinger J. R., Isbell L. M., & Clore G. L. (2014). The affective control of thought: Malleable, not fixed.Psychology Review, 121(4), 600-618. [27] Isbell L. M., Rovenpor D. R., & Lair E. C. (2016). The impact of negative emotions on self-concept abstraction depends on accessible information processing styles.Emotion, 16(7), 1040-1049. [28] Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146-159. [29] Li X., Li Y., Wang X., Fan X., Tong W., & Hu W. J. (2020). The effects of emotional valence on insight problem solving in global-local processing: An ERP study.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 155, 194-203. [30] Liu, S., Peng, & Ming. (2020). Does scope of attention affect creativity? Testing the attentional priming hypothesis.The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54(2), 423-435. [31] Martin, L. L., & Stoner, P. (1996). Mood as input: What we think about how we feel determines how we think. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation(pp. 279-301). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. [32] Mastria S., Agnoli S., & Corazza G. E. (2019). How does emotion influence the creativity evaluation of exogenous alternative ideas?PloS One, 14(7), e0219298. [33] Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception.Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383. [34] Runco M. A.(1991). Divergent thinking. Ablex Publishing Corporation Norwood.. [35] Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity.American Psychologist, 51(7), 677-688. [36] Vosburg, S. K. (1998). The effects of positive and negative mood on divergent-thinking performance.Creativity Research Journal, 11(2), 165-172. [37] Ward, T. B. (2008). The role of domain knowledge in creative generation.Learning and Individual Differences, 18(4), 363-366. [38] Zmigrod S., Zmigrod L., & Hommel B. (2015). Zooming into creativity: Individual differences in attentional global-local biases are linked to creative thinking.Frontiers in Psychology, (6), 1647.