丁凤琴, 王小芳. 净脏启动对负性道德情绪词加工的影响:具身隐喻视角[J]. 心理研究, 2023, 16(5): 419-428.
DING Fengqin, WANG Xiaofang. Influence of clean-dirty priming on the processing of negative moral emotion words: Perspective of embodied metaphor. Psychological Research, 2023, 16(5): 419-428.
[1] 北京语言学院编印. (1986). 现代汉语频率词典. 北京语言学院. [2] 常行, 冯申梅, 陈玮, 甘海英, 朱金富. (2020). 厌恶启动后水平空间隐喻加工的时间特征:来自事件相关电位(ERPs)的证据.中国健康心理学杂志, 28(2), 266-269. [3] 陈英和, 白柳, 李龙凤. (2015). 道德情绪的特点、发展及其对行为的影响.心理与行为研究, 13(5), 627-636. [4] 丁凤琴, 王喜梅, 刘钊. (2017). 道德概念净脏隐喻及其对道德判断的影响.心理发展与教育, 33(6), 666-674. [5] 贾宁, 陈换娟, 鲁忠义. (2018). 句子启动范式下的道德概念空间隐喻:匹配抑制还是匹配易化?心理发展与教育, 34(5), 541-547. [6] 李昳, 陆桂芝, 李勇. (2018). 自我关注与社交焦虑:负面评价恐惧的中介与关系型自我构念的调节.心理科学, 41(5), 1261-1267. [7] 梁晓燕, 马虹, 范红霞. (2012). 大学生道德情绪对身体洁净内隐认知影响的研究.教育研究与实验, (5), 73-78. [8] 李莹, 张灿, 王悦. (2019). 道德情绪在道德隐喻映射中的作用及其神经机制.心理科学进展, 27(7), 1224-1231. [9] 鲁忠义, 贾利宁, 翟冬雪. (2017). 道德概念垂直空间隐喻理解中的映射:双向性及不平衡性.心理学报, 49(2), 186-196. [10] 罗一丽, 张辉. (2016). 英汉道德隐喻对比研究:基于语料库“肮脏(dirt) /干净(cleanliness)”的隐喻表征.外语研究, 33(6), 18-24. [11] 钱铭怡, 戚健俐. (2002). 大学生羞耻和内疚差异的对比研究.心理学报, 34(6), 626-633. [12] 王博韬, 魏萍. (2021). 道德情绪:探寻道德与创造力关系的新视角.心理科学进展, 29(2), 268-275. [13] 王继瑛, 叶浩生, 苏得权. ( 2018). 身体动作与语义加工:具身隐喻的视角.心理学探新, 38(1), 15-19. [14] 解晴楠, 任维聪, 王汉林. (2019). 善与恶的重量意象:道德概念的重量隐喻研究.心理研究, 12(3), 227-232. [15] 颜志雄, 燕良轼, 范伟, 丁道群, 邹霞. (2014). 身体清洁启动后道德纯洁词加工的时间特征:来自ERPs的证据.心理与行为研究, 12(5), 609-615. [16] 叶浩生. (2017). 具身认知的原理与应用. 北京:商务出版社. [17] 殷融, 叶浩生. (2014). 道德概念的黑白隐喻表征及其对道德认知的影响.心理学报, 46(9), 1331-1346. [18] 张裕鼎, 张翀, 吴鹏. (2020). 道德概念的软硬隐喻表征及其对道德判断的影响.教育研究与实验, (4), 33-39. [19] 赵岩, 伍麟. (2019). 具身认知视角下的道德隐喻表征.心理学探新, 39(4), 308-313. [20] 周详, 杨治良, 郝雁丽. (2007). 理性学习的局限:道德情绪理论对道德养成的启示.道德与文明, (3), 57-60. [21] 朱金富, 陈玮, 蒲明慧, 冯申梅. (2018). 厌恶启动后垂直空间隐喻加工的时间特征.中华行为医学与脑科学杂志, 27(3), 266-271. [22] Ackerman, J. M., Nocera, C. C, & Bargh, J. A. (2010). Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science, 328, 1712-1715. [23] Chapman H. A., Kim D. A., Susskind J. M., & Anderson A. K. (2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust.Science, 323, 1179-1180. [24] Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 665-697. [25] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.Behavior Research Methods, 39 (2), 175-191. [26] Frimer, J. A. & Sinclair, L. (2016). Moral heroes look up and to the right.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(3), 400-410. [27] Holland R. W., Hendriks M., & Aarts H. (2005). Smells like clean spirit: Nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior.Psychological Science, 16(9), 689-693. [28] Holzman, J. B., & Valentiner, D. P. (2016). Self-focused attention affects subsequent processing of positive (but not negative) performance appraisals.Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 50, 295-302. [29] Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors we live by. London: The university of Chicago press. [30] Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Dirty hands and dirty mouths: Embodiment of the moral-purity metaphor is specific to the motor modality involved in moral transgression.Psychological Science, 21(10), 1423-1425. [31] Liljenquist K., Zhong C. B., & Galinsky A. D. (2010). The smell of virtue clean scents promote reciprocity and charity.Psychological Science, 21(3), 381-383. [32] Meier B. P., Robinson M. D., & Clore G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear white.Psychological Science, 15(2), 82-87. [33] Meier B. P., Sellbom M., & Wygant D. B. (2007). Failing to take the moral high ground: Psychopathy and the vertical representation of morality.Personality and Individual Differences, 43 (4), 757-767. [34] Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 1002-1005. [35] Schnall S., Benton J., & Harvey S. (2008). With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments.University of Plymouth, 19(12), 1219-1222. [36] Schnall S., Haidt J., Clore G. L., & Jordan A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1096-1109. [37] Sherman, G. D., & Clore, G. L. (2009). The color of sin white and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution.Psychological Science, 20, 1019-1025. [38] Sheikh S., Botindari L., & White E. (2013). Embodied metaphors and emotions in the moralization of restrained eating practices.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 509-513. [39] Tang H., Lu X., Su R., Liang Z., Mai X., & Liu C. (2017). Washing away your sins in the brain: Physical cleaning and priming of cleaning recruit different brain networks after moral threat.Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience, 12 (7), 1149-1158. [40] Tangney J. P., Miller, R. S, Flicker, L., & Barlow D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1256-1269. [41] Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625-636. [42] Winkielman P., Niedenthal P., Wielgosz J., Eelen J., & Kavanagh L. C. (2015). Embodiment of cognition and emotion. APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology:1. Attitudes and Social Cognition, M. Mikulincer and P. R. Shaver (Editors-in-Chief). [43] Xu H., Bègue L., & Bushman B. J. (2014). Washing the guilt away: Effects of personal versus vicarious cleansing on guilty feelings and prosocial behavior.Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1-5. [44] Zhai D., Guo Y., & Lu Z. (2018). A dual mechanism of cognition and emotion in processing moral-vertical metaphors.Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1-9. [45] Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing.Science, 313 (5792), 1451-1452. [46] Zhong C. B., Strejcek B., & Sivanathan N. (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 859-862.