|
|
Review on the experimental tasks of reactive aggression |
QUAN Fangying1,2,3, LEI Xiaofang4, LIANG Binqi1,2,3, WANG Lu1,2,3, XIA Lingxiang5, YU Xinxin1,2,3 |
1 Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Guangxi Normal University, Guilin 541004; 2 Research Center for the Development of Guangxi Ethnic Education, Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences in Guangxi Universities, Guilin 541004; 3 Guangxi University and College Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Applied Psychology, Guilin 541004; 4 Guangxi Vocational College of Water Resources and Electric Power,Nanning 53000; 5 Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715 |
|
|
Abstract Reactive aggression is a major subtype of aggression. The evaluation methods mainly include questionnaire and experimental tasks. The questionnaire is used to measure trait reactive aggression and the experimental task is used to measure state reactive aggression. This study mainly reviews the experimental tasks of state reactive aggression. Competitive Reactive Time Task (CRTT), Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (PSAP), Social Fairness Game (SFG) and Social Network Aggression Task (SNAT) belong to the experimental tasks with provoked stimulus. Hot Sauce Paradigm (HSP), Voodoo Doll Task (VDT), Tangram Help/Hurt Task (THHT) and VR aggression task belong to the experimental tasks without provoked stimulus, researchers need to operate additional provoked stimulus before using these tasks to measure reactive aggression. Finally, some suggestions on developing the experimental tasks of reactive aggression and improving the evaluation effect of reactive aggression are proposed.
|
|
|
|
|
[1] 江琦, 侯璐璐, 邱江, 李长燃, 王焕贞. (2018). 尾状核—眶部内侧前额叶的功能连接与反应性攻击的关系: 基于静息态功能磁共振研究.心理学报, 50(6), 655-666. [2] 孙连荣, 杨治良. (2010). 攻击性的实验研究范式.心理科学, 33(6), 1436-1438. [3] 杨光辉, 周宗奎, 王宁丹. (2010). 攻击的结构与测量范式.心理科学, 33(4), 922-924. [4] 杨如姣. (2020). 反应性攻击的激惹—攻击任务开发与检验. 西南大学硕士学位论文. [5] 赵永萍, 赵玉芳, 黎岳庭. (2019). 校园欺凌测量工具的多视角分析与选用.西南大学学报, 45(4), 120-121. [6] Achterberg M., van Duijvenvoorde A. C., Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J., & Crone E. A. (2016). Control your anger! The neural basis of aggression regulation in response to negative social feedback.Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(5), 712-720. [7] Achterberg M., van Duijvenvoorde A. C., van der Meulena M., Euser S., Bakermans-kranenburg M. J., & Crone E. A. (2017). The neural and behavioral correlates of social evaluation in childhood.Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 107-117. [8] Achterberg M., Van Duijvenvoorde A. C., van IJzendoorn M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J., & Crone E. A. (2020). Longitudinal changes in DLPFC activation during childhood are related to decreased aggression following social rejection.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(15), 8602-8610. [9] Anderson, C. A., & Murphy, C. R. (2003). Violent video games and aggressive behavior in young women.Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression, 29(5), 423-429. [10] Barlett C. P., Harris R. J., & Bruey C. (2008). The effect of the amount of blood in a violent video game on aggression, hostility, and arousal.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 539-546. [11] Boccadoro S., Wagels L., Henn A. T., Hüpen P., Graben L., Raine A., & Neuner I. (2021). Proactive vs. reactive aggression within two modified versions of the Taylor Aggression Paradigm.Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 1-15. [12] Cherek, D. R. (1981). Effects of smoking different doses of nicotine on human aggressive behavior.Psychopharmacology, 75, 339-345. [13] Cherek D. R., Lane S. D., Dougherty D. M., Moeller F. G., & White S. (2000). Laboratory and questionnaire measures of aggression among female parolees with violent or nonviolent histories.Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression, 26(4), 291-307. [14] Chester, D. S., & Dzierzewski, J. M. (2020). Sour sleep, sweet revenge? Aggressive pleasure as a potential mechanism underlying poor sleep quality's link to aggression.Emotion, 20(5), 842. [15] Dambacher F., Sack A. T., Lobbestael J., Arntz A., Brugman S., & Schuhmann T. (2015). Out of control: Evidence for anterior insula involvement in motor impulsivity and reactive aggression.Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(4), 508-516. [16] DeWall C. N., Finkel E. J., Lambert N. M., Slotter E. B., Bodenhausen G. V., Pond Jr R. S., ... & Fincham, F. D. (2013). The voodoo doll task: Introducing and validating a novel method for studying aggressive inclinations.Aggressive Behavior, 39(6), 419-439. [17] David S. C., & Emily N. L., (2019). Validating a standardized approach to the Taylor Aggression Paradigm.Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 3-33. [18] Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1146. [19] Ellis J. D., Grekin E. R., Dekeyser D., & Partridge T. (2021). Using an online platform to administer the single-session point subtraction aggression paradigm: An initial examination of feasibility and validity.Assessment, 28(1), 310-321. [20] Elson M., Mohseni M. R., Breuer J., Scharkow M., & Quandt, T. (2014). Press CRTT to measure aggressive behavior: The unstandardized use of the competitive reaction time task in aggression research Psychological Assessment The unstandardized use of the competitive reaction time task in aggression research. Psychological Assessment, 26, 419-432. [21] Ferguson C. J., Smith S., Miller-Stratton H., Fritz S., & Heinrich E. (2008). Aggression in the laboratory: Problems with the validity of the modified Taylor Competitive Reaction Time Test as a measure of aggression in media violence studies.Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 17(1), 118-132. [22] Gentile D. A., Anderson C. A., Yukawa S., Ihori N., Saleem M., Ming L. K., ... & Sakamoto A. (2009). The effects of prosocial video games on prosocial behaviors: International evidence from correlational, longitudinal, and experimental studies.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(6), 752-763. [23] Geniole S. N., Cunningham C. E., Keyes A. E., Busseri M. A., & McCormick C. M. (2015). Costly retaliation is promoted by threats to resources in women and threats to status in men.Aggressive Behavior, 41(6), 515-525. [24] Geniole S. N., MacDonell E. T., & McCormick C. M. (2017). The Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm as a laboratory tool for investigating the neuroendocrinology of aggression and competition.Hormones and Behavior, 92, 103-116. [25] Jiang Q., Hou L., Wang H., & Li C. (2018). The effect of cognitive reappraisal on reactive aggression: An fMRI study.Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1-12. [26] Kaltsouni E., Fisher P. M., Dubol M., Hustad S., Lanzenberger R., Frokjaer V. G., ... & Sundstr?m-Poromaa I. (2021). Brain reactivity during aggressive response in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder treated with a selective progesterone receptor modulator.Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(8), 1460-1467. [27] Konzok J., Henze G. I., Kreuzpointner L., Peter H. L., Giglberger M., Bärtl C., ... & Kudielka, B. M. (2022). Dissociation of behavioral and neural responses to provocation during reactive aggression in healthy adults with high versus low externalization.Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 1, 1-15. [28] Krämer U. M., Jansma H., Tempelmann C., & Münte T. F. (2007). Tit-for-tat: The neural basis of reactive aggression.NeuroImage, 38(1), 203-211. [29] Liang L. H., Brown D. J., Lian H., Hanig S., Ferris D. L., & Keeping L. M. (2018). Righting a wrong: Retaliation on a voodoo doll symbolizing an abusive supervisor restores justice.The Leadership Quarterly, 29(4), 443-456. [30] Lieberman J. D., Solomon S., Greenberg J., & McGregor H. A. (1999). A hot new way to measure aggression: Hot sauce allocation.Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression, 25(5), 331-348. [31] Lotze M., Veit R., Anders S., & Birbaumer N. (2007). Evidence for a different role of the ventral and dorsal medial prefrontal cortex for social reactive aggression: An interactive fMRI study.Neuroimage, 34(1), 470-478. [32] Lobbestael, J., & Cima, M. J. (2021). Virtual reality for aggression assessment: The development and preliminary results of two virtual reality tasks to assess reactive and proactive aggression in males.Brain Sciences, 11(12), 1653. [33] MacDonald K. J., Lustig K. A., Geniole S. N., McCormick C. M., & Cote K. A. (2019). Sleep restriction alters reactive aggressive behavior and its relationship with sex hormones.Aggressive Behavior, 45(2), 193-205. [34] McCarthy R. J., Crouch J. L., Basham A. R., Milner J. S., & Skowronski J. J. (2016). Validating the voodoo doll task as a proxy for aggressive parenting behavior.Psychology of Violence, 6(1), 135. [35] Papousek I., Reiter-Scheidl K., Lackner H. K., Weiss E. M., Perchtold-Stefan C. M., & Aydin N. (2020). The impacts of the presence of an unfamiliar dog on emerging adults' physiological and behavioral responses following social exclusion.Behavioral Sciences, 10(12), 191. [36] Randy J. McCarthy & Malte Elson. (2018). A conceptual review of lab-based aggression paradigms.Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 21-22. [37] Saleem M., Barlett C. P., Anderson C. A., & Hawkins I. (2017). Helping and hurting others: Person and situation effects on aggressive and prosocial behavior as assessed by the Tangram task.Aggressive Behavior, 43(2), 133-146. [38] Skibsted A. P., Cunha-Bang S. D., Carré J. M., Hansen A. E., Beliveau V., Knudsen G. M., & Fisher P. M. (2017). Aggression related brain function assessed with the Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm in fMRI.Aggressive Behavior, 43(6), 601-610. [39] Stanford M. S., Houston R. J., Mathias C. W., Villemarette-Pittman N. R., Helfritz L. E., & Conklin S. M. (2003). Characterizing aggressive behavior.Assessment, 10(2), 183-190. [40] Taylor, S. P. (1967). Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and the tendency to inhibit aggression.Journal of Personality, 35(2), 297-310. [41] Tedeschi, J. T., & Quigley, B. M. (1996). Limitations of laboratory paradigms for studying aggression.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 1(2), 163-177. [42] Warburton, W. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2019). The competitive reaction time task: The development and scientific utility of a flexible laboratory aggression paradigm.Aggressive Behavior, 45(4), 1-8. [43] West S. J., Hyatt C. S., Miller J. D., & Chester D. S. (2020). p-Curve analysis of the Taylor Aggression Paradigm: Estimating evidentiary value and statistical power across 50 years of research.Aggressive Behavior, 47(2), 1-11. [44] White-Ajmani, M. L., & Bursik, K. (2014). Situational context moderates the relationship between moral disengagement and aggression.Psychology of Violence, 4(1), 90. [45] White S. F., Brislin S. J., Sinclair S., & Blair J. R. (2013). Punishing unfairness: Rewarding or the organization of a reactively aggressive response?Human Brain Mapping, 35(5), 2137-2147. [46] White S. F., VanTieghem M., Brislin S. J., Sypher I., Sinclair S., Pine D. S., ... & Blair, R. J. R. (2016). Neural correlates of the propensity for retaliatory behavior in youths with disruptive behavior disorders.American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(3), 282-290. [47] Yeager D. S., Trzesniewski K. H., & Dweck C. S. (2013). An implicit theories of personality intervention reduces adolescent aggression in response to victimization and exclusion.Child Development, 84(3), 970-988. |
|
|
|